IS THERE A DEFICIENT BRAIN CHEMISTRY IN ADDICTS?
If everyone with a brain can become an addict, why
are there (relatively) so few addicts? Could there be a unique group of
people whose pleasure circuits are abnormal
in some way so that these drugs feel particularly good? Or could there be a
group of people whose pleasure circuits don't work very well, so
that they are inclined to drink alcohol, smoke, or take cocaine to feel normal?
There are probably people in each of these categories. In studying these questions in human addicts,
there is a real "chicken and
egg" problem. If brain function is abnormal, it is impossible to know whether the abnormality was caused by years of
substance abuse or was present before.
This is one challenge about the aforementioned dopamine receptor finding. Some scientists have tried to
solve this problem by studying the
children of alcoholics. There are certain EEG (brain wave) changes that have been noted in some alcoholics
and in their sons. However, we don't
really understand the significance of this EEG anomaly yet. The only way to be
sure is to study these children until they become adults to see if this difference predicted alcoholism.
Such studies are underway, but they
take a long time. We can do these experiments in animals, and we have found that even with free access to
cocaine, only a certain percentage of
animals (about a fifth) progress to the stage of compulsive use. Are these differences due to a deficient gene that could simply be repaired? The mapping of the human genome has
really speeded up the search for genes
related to addiction as well as other diseases. Many candidates have been identified. Some are specific to
specific addictions. A variant of one
gene for the receptor through which ethanol acts is associated with alcoholism,
and a variant for a receptor that narcotics act upon is associated with narcotic addiction. Others, like
the dopamine D2 receptor, are related
to all addictions. Others have been surprises. One of the best
genetic "predictors' of nicotine dependence is
a gene that controls the breakdown of nicotine in the liver—not anything
related to brain function at all. Finally, there are genes that seem to
protect people from addictions.
Two genes involved in alcohol degradation fit into
this category (see the
chapter on alcohol). So, as many scientists
predicted, drug addiction is a complicated disorder that can involve many genes.
Can we fix the affected genes? Not yet. Do we want to? Because most or
these genes affect normal brain activities, we are not even vaguely close to
knowing if changing them would treat addiction without causing other
troubles. And even if we
could, the ethical questions raised by such
manipulations are huge. Finally, it is important to realize that biology is not destiny. People are more
than bags of genes that produce behavior. They are influenced bytheir
environment and can control their behavior voluntarily. Simply possessing a particular
gene that has been found in the brains of some alco‑ holics
does not mean that an individual must become an alcoholic. If he or she abstains from
alcohol, for one thing, there will never be a problem.
Maybe these slightly
abnormal genes provide some benefit to the person that we don't fully understand. On the other hand,
people with no genetic predisposition
may experience such traumatic life circumstances (being sexually abused during childhood, for example) that
they develop compulsive use of alcohol or other substances in an attempt to
self-medicate their psychological trauma. The bottom line is that everyone with
a brain can become an addict. Given
the diversity of human brains, it is likely that some people will find the experience more compelling than others, but we have not really defined exactly what brain
chemistry leads to this vulnerability
yet.